• Common marmosets use body posture as multi-functional signal to solicit, maintain, and modify social play

    Authors: Adriaense J. E.C., Ringen E. J., Ohashi A., Burkart J. M. (2024)

    ABSTRACT 

    Social play is a highly active social interaction, characterized by rapid exchanges of various behaviors with multiple partners. Many primates use bodily expressions during social play, yet the potential signaling function of these expressions remains unclear. This study investigated whether common marmosets (Callithrix jacchus) use body posture as signal to regulate play. We recorded play within three captive common marmoset family groups using multiple cameras simultaneously to capture the fast-paced and high frequency behaviors. Three distinct signals (i.e. supine, hide, stalk) and six distinct play types (i.e. wrestle, chase, pounce, touch, catch, pull) were identified. We used a multi-state timeto-event model to analyze the sequences of play, including short- and long-term transitions between differentstates (i.e. signal, play, or rest/nothing). Our data-driven approach accounted for uncertainty in the duration of play bouts, using probabilistic classification rather than arbitrary bout thresholds. The resulting classifications allowed us to assess the social function of signals by comparing play behavior to a resting state baseline. We found that the presence of a signal: (1) increases the probability to play; (2) extends the duration of play; (3) leads to more diverse play; and (4) increases the probability of play fighting. Marmosets also show turn-taking of signaling and initiating subsequential play. These results show that marmosets use postures as communicative signals to initiate and change play dynamics, and thereby establish a mutual understanding of the joint action. The two-fold contribution of this study concerns novel analytical methods and a deeper conceptual understanding of primate communication. Play and its signals are important elementsin the evolution of language, and our research contributes to its further understanding.

    Continue reading >>>

    Download
  • The ontogeny of play in a highly cooperative monkey, the common marmoset

    Authors: Godard A. M., Burkart J. M., Brügger R. K. (2024)

    Abstract
    Play is widespread in mammals. It is mostly observed in juveniles and has been subdivided in three categories, social, locomotor/rotational, and object play. Although there is no strict consensus on its ultimate function, the dominant idea is that through play juveniles acquire social, technical and cognitive skills for their adult life. In certain species, however, adults remain playful especially with immatures. This pattern can be observed in particular when same-age play partners for immatures are lacking and if adults also invest in caretaking. We studied the ontogeny of play in cooperatively breeding common marmoset twins from the age of two to six months. Social play increased with age and was by far the most prevalent category. Play partners varied with age. Before 19 weeks old, immatures played 54% of the time on average with either one of their parents (in a dyad) and 29% on average after 19 weeks old. Thus, despite the constant presence of a twin, adult-immature play remained considerable, with equal contributions by mothers and fathers and no trade-offs with other care-taking behaviours for either of the parents. However, parents avoided playing simultaneously, presumably to avoid periods with no one vigilant. Together these results show that parents are important play partners for marmoset infants, fathers and mothers alike.

    Continue reading >>>

  • Planning abilities of wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in tool-using contexts

    Authors: Musgrave, S., Koni, D., Morgan, D. et al. (2023)

    Abstract

    Planning is a type of problem solving in which a course of future action is devised via mental computation. Potential advantages of planning for tool use include reduced effort to gather tools, closer alignment to an efficient tool design, and increased foraging efficiency. Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes troglodytes) in the Goualougo Triangle use a variety of different types of tools. We hypothesized that procurement strategy (brought to the termite nest, manufactured or acquired at the termite nest, or borrowed from others) reflects planning for current needs, with tool transport behavior varying by tool type and by age and sex class. It is also possible that chimpanzees anticipate the need for tools at future times, which would be evidenced by transporting multiple tool types for a sequential task. One year of video recordings at termite nests were systematically screened for tool procurement; data comprised 299 tool procurement events across 66 chimpanzees. In addition, we screened video recordings of leaf sponging and honey gathering, which resulted in another 38 procurement events. Fishing probes, which are typically used during a single visit, were typically transported to termite nests, while puncturing tools, which are durable and remain on site, were more often acquired at termite nests. Most tools transported in multiples were fishing probes, perhaps in anticipation that a single probe might not last through an entire foraging bout or might be transferred to another chimpanzee. We further documented that chimpanzees transported tool sets, comprising multiple different tool types used in sequence. Mature chimpanzees transported tools more often than did immatures. These observations suggest that chimpanzees plan tool use flexibly, reflecting the availability of raw materials and the likelihood that specific tool types will be needed for particular tasks. Developmental studies and further integration of behavioral, spatial, and archaeological data will help to illuminate the decision making and time depth of planning associated with tool technologies in living primates and hominin ancestors.

    Keywords: cognition, tool use, tool manufacture, tool set, termite fish

    Continue reading >>>

  • Systematic Evaluation of Different Fresh Cow Monitoring Procedures

    Authors: König F, Hancock A, Wunderlich C, Klawitter M, Breuer T, Simoni A, Weimar K, Drillich M, Iwersen M. (2023)

    Abstract

    Establishing fresh cow monitoring procedures is considered beneficial for cow health, welfare, and productivity. However, they are time consuming and require the cows to be locked up, which restricts their natural behavior. In this study, different fresh cow monitoring procedures were evaluated. Two experiments were conducted to determine: (1) the duration of various examinations and treatments; (2) the time cows remain locked up in headlocks; and (3) the proportion of examination and treatment times relative to the total headlock time. In advance, standard operating procedures were established. Three veterinarians conducted the examinations and treatments based on changes in milk yield, clinical symptoms, and alarms by an accelerometer system. The headlock time was evaluated for three workflow strategies, which differed in the order of examinations and treatments. To determine the duration, cameras were installed, and the video footage was analyzed. The examinations lasted between 1 and 227 s, and the cows were locked up in headlocks between 0.01 and 1.76 h. The lock-up times differed significantly among the three strategies, as well as the proportion. This study provides information that can be used as a basis for the development of time-efficient strategies, and to minimize the impact on cows’ time budgets.

    Keywords: dairy cow; animal welfare; animal management; precision livestock management

    Continue reading >>>

  • The impact of cage dividers on mouse aggression, dominance and hormone levels

    Authors: Streiff C., Herrera A., Voelkl B., Palme R., Würbel H., Novak J. (2024)

    Abstract 

    Home cage aggression in group-housed male mice is a major welfare concern and may compromise animal research. Conventional cages prevent flight or retreat from sight,increasing the risk that agonistic encounters will result in injury. Moreover, depending onsocial rank, mice vary in their phenotype, and these effects seem highly variable and dependent on the social context. Interventions that reduce aggression, therefore, may reduce not only injuries and stress, but also variability between cage mates. Here we housed male mice (Balb/c and SWISS, group sizes of three and five) with or without partial cage dividers for two months. Mice were inspected for wounding weekly and home cages were recorded during housing and after 6h isolation housing, to assess aggression and assign individual social ranks. Fecal boli and fur were collected to quantify steroid levels. We found no evidence that the provision of cage dividers improves the welfare of group housed male mice; The prevalence of injuries and steroid levels was similar between the two housing conditions and aggression was reduced only in Balb/c strain. However, mice housed with cage dividers developed less despotic hierarchies and had more stable social ranks. We also found a relationship between hormone levels and social rank depending on housing type. Therefore, addition of cage dividers may play a role in stabilizing social ranks and modulating the activation of hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal (HPA) and hypothalamic-pituitary-gonadal (HPG)axes, thus reducing phenotypicvariability between mice of different ranks.

    Continue reading >>>

  • Rearing of female calves and young cattle in agricultural enterprises Part III Normal behaviour of calves in motherless, intensive rearing

    Authors: Schuldt A., and Dinse R. (2023)

    Abstract
    The studies on the behaviour of female German Holstein calves in intensive off-cow breeding were carried out in an agricultural enterprise in Mecklen-burg-Vorpommern. The calves were kept in hutches with a run to the end of week 4 at the latest. The calves were moved from week 3 to subsequent group housing in a pen with a litter-covered lying area and concrete running area. The maximum age difference within a group was 4 weeks. The calves were reared with a maximum daily allowance of 12 L CMR-feed up to day 49 and weaned on day 106. Over the entire feeding period, CMR powder (50% skim milk fraction) was used at a concentration of 160 g/L water. Hay and dry total mixed ration (TMR) were used as supplementary feed provided fresh daily ad libitum in racks and troughs with TMR gradually replacing the dry TMR during weaning. The calf pen was fitted with elements to investigate which activity options were preferred by the animals. A tyre freely hanging on a rope with a ball (ball), a cattle brush for calves (brush) and board with four chains (chains) were in-stalled in the pen. It was also investigated whether the calves accepted dum-my teats. The behaviour of the calves was recorded continuously over 24 hours with video cameras from the day they moved into the barn to the day they moved out. In total, data from 13 calves in five rounds over 212 days with 4,569.5 hours of video recordings were coded with the Interact program from Mangold and statistically processed and analysed by week of life (week 3–7 and week 8–15) or day of life (day 50 to 105) with Interact and Excel 2019 MSO from Microsoft (Version 2207). The analysis of behaviour with maximum allowance (12 L CMR-feed per animal up to day 49, n = 9 calves) was carried out for the functional areas rest, food and water-intake, social behaviour (calf–calf contacts), and ‘other activi-ties’, which include exploratory, play, locomotion and elimination behaviours. Exploration (licking of objects) and play were coded, while locomotion (stand-ing without activity, slow and fast running, jumping, galloping) and elimination were calculated from the difference in the duration of the activities and the behaviour coded in this phase and summarised as locomotion behaviour. The use of toys, visits without CMR-feed-intake and sucking activities (cross-sucking, sucking on dummy teats) were assessed separately. From the studies, an ethogram for calves in off-cow rearing can be derived: Circadian rhythm Calves develop a distinct circadian rhythm that underlies almost all behav-iours. The behaviour at night, that is, from 12 midnight to about 6:00 a.m., alternates between long resting phases with brief wake phases for defecation and urination as well as the intake of CMR-feed, supplementary feed and/or water. During the day, from 6:00 a.m. to 12 midnight, the calves alternate between active and resting phases, which are considerably shorter than over-night. Resting behaviour The calves seek out protected places to rest. After a brief interruption, they often lay down in the same place where they had stood up. Young calves rest daily for 14 to 18 hours for an average of 30 to 45 minutes during the day and for 60 to 180 minutes at night. In the mornings and evenings the resting times are somewhat shorter than over midday. Feeding behaviour The sucking behaviour at the dispensing station corresponds to the natural sucking at a cow in terms of the posture, bunting and tail movements as well as the mean number of four to five meals per day. The individual meals last on average four to six minutes with a rising trend. Visits to the dispensing station without milk intake occur briefly up to day 49 and last less than one minute on average. In the week when the calves are adjusting to the free feed-intake, up to four of these blind visits per animal and day are tolerable, thereafter a daily average of one to three up to weaning. In the weaning phase slightly longer blind visits in an increasing number can be considered normal if they do not significantly exceed a daily average of 10 per animal. Up to the end of week 7, calves frequently ingest supplementary feed over the course of the day with only a short eating duration. The number increases to about 20 meals per day and the duration to about three minutes per meal. The calves drink water from the start for about one minute and the frequency and duration increase with the intake of supplementary feed. Individual animals have a distinct sucking need that cannot be satisfied through the feed-intake even with the highest CMR-feed-allowances. If these are isolated cases, cross-sucking of another calf can be tolerated. Sucking calves from a maternal sucking family should be excluded from breeding, however. Dummy teats are accepted by the calves but cannot prevent cross-sucking and are used less than a moveable toy. Social behaviour Calves smell and lick each other, play together and rest closely with one an-other. Hierarchical disputes are not observed before weaning. There were no signs in the behaviour of the young calves that indicate that the calves are stressed by the absence of the mother–child relationship. Locomotion and play behaviour Calves run, jump and gallop around, often together and encouraging each other. Intense activities are often observed in the evening hours. Playful headbutting starts as early as week 2. Calves prefer to use moveable objects that they can make swing as toys. These toys are licked, sucked or sniffed, often by several calves at the same time. Locomotion and play behaviour can be summarised as ‘other behav-iours’ to evaluate the well-being of calves. The daily average of the percent-age of the active time spent on these behaviours should be at least 80% up to weaning. When weaning, the percentage decreases because of the increas-ing intake of supplementary feed but should not be less than 60% of the ac-tivities. Weaning Moderate weaning is recommended from the perspective of animal behav-iour so that the animals rest for long periods, only make few unrewarded visits to the dispensing station and are ensured of having a high supplementary feed-intake upon weaning. Because the maximum milk replacer allowance must be provided up to day 49, this results in a recommended weaning age of at least 105 days.

    Continue reading >>>

  • Usage of outdoor runs and defaecation behaviour of fattening pigs

    Authors: Höne U., Krause E. T., Bussemas R., Traulsen I., Schrader L. (2023)

    Abstract:
    Access to an outdoor run improves the housing conditions of fattening pigs by an enlarged space allowance, access to different climatic conditions and environmental stimulation. One central conflicting issue regarding outdoor runs is the emission of harmful gases such as ammonia resulting from faeces and urine. However, by establishing appropriate functional areas in fattening pens, the area soiled by faeces and urine can potentially be reduced. Here, we investigated the usage of an outdoor run and the frequencies and locations of defaecation behaviour of fattening pigs kept in groups of ten in eight pens. Usage of the outdoor run was registered by scan sampling at the group level in three observation periods (pigs at 16, 19, and 22 weeks of age). Furthermore, in each pen, two focal pigs were individually marked and continuously observed for 24 h in each observation period to record defaecation behaviour. At the group level, the fattening pigs used the outdoor run mainly during the daytime between 06:00 h and 20:00 h. A total of 99.4% of defaecation events occurred in the outdoor run, most often in the corner next to the wall of the barn. On average, 11.08 ( ± SD 3.06) defaecation events per pig per day were recorded, whereby the time of day significantly affected the relative frequencies of defaecation behaviour (GLMER, X12 = 4.11, P < 0.043). Fattening pigs defaecated more often during the daytime than during the nighttime. The number of daily defaecations decreased with age. In pens as used in our study, fattening pigs perform nearly all defaecations in the outdoor run and within a small area. This may enable technical possibilities to reduce ammonia emissions in pig pens with outdoor runs, such as regular cleaning of dunging areas by an automatic floor scraper.

    Keywords:
    Organic pig farming; Fattening pigs; Outdoor runs; Elimination areas; Defaecation behaviour

    Continue reading >>>

  • Effects of sounds of different quality on the behaviour and heart beat parameters of goats

    Authors: Johns, J., Patt, A., Hillmann, E. (2015)

    In alpine regions, bells are used to relocate free-ranging grazers like cows and goats. Considering that goats have a well-developed hearing capacity, sounds (e.g. a chime of a bell) mayact as stressors depending on their characteristics. The aim of this study was to test whethera non-uniform sound (chime of a bell) varying in amplitude and frequency and a uniform sound (sinusoidal tone) with continuously increasing amplitude and constant frequency lead to stress responses in terms of behaviour and heart beat. Twenty-nine goats were tested individually in a test arena in two sessions, each lasting five consecutive days with one trial per day. A day before the first trial, reference values were collected without playback. During the following five trials, playbacks were conducted. Differences in behaviour and heart beat parameters between test and reference values were analysed by using generalised linear mixed-effects models. During the first trial, the relative feeding duration was decreased and the relative alertness duration was increased during both stimuli, but more when goats were exposed to the non-uniform than the uniform sound. For both stimuli, the relative feeding duration increased (trial × stimulus: P = 0.05) and the relative alertness duration decreased (trial × stimulus: P = 0.004) from the first to the fifth trial but returned to the levels of the reference values sooner when goats were exposed to the uniform than the non-uniform sound. Cardiac activity was not affected by the stimuli. Altogether, the chime of a bell led to higher behavioural arousal than the uniform sinusoidal tone, indicating a potential of the chime to being more aversive to goats than a uniform sound. With repeated exposure to the stimuli, goats habituated to both stimuli, but habituation was faster to the sinusoidal sound than to the chime of a bell. Free-ranging goats in alpine regions usually are equipped with bells 24 h a day during the summer season. Thus, the question arises whether the long-term exposure to the chime of a bell might have negative effects on animal welfare.

    Learn more >>>

  • Does nest size matter to laying hens?

    Authors: Ringgenberg, N., Fröhlich, E., Harlander-Matauschek, A., Würbel, H., Roth, B. (2014)

    Laying hens in loose housing systems have access to group-nests which provide space for several hens at a time to lay their eggs. They are thus rather large and the trend in the industry is to further increase the size of these nests. Though practicality is important for the producer, group-nests should also cater to the egg-laying behaviour of hens to promote good welfare. One of the factors playing a role in the attractiveness of a nest is the amount of enclosure: hens prefer more enclosure when having a choice between different nest types. The aim of this study was to investigate if hens prefer smaller group-nests to lay their eggs given that they may seem more enclosed than larger nests.

    The relative preference of groups of laying hens for two nest sizes – 0.43 m2 vs. 0.86 m2 – was tested in a free-access choice test. The experiment was conducted in two consecutive trials with 100 hens each. They were housed from 18 to 36 weeks of age in five groups of 20 animals and had access to two commercial group-nests differing in internal size only. We counted eggs daily as a measure of nest preference. At 28 and 36 weeks of age, videos were taken of the pens and inside the nests on one day during the first 5 h of lights-on. The nest videos were used to record the number of hens per nest and their behaviour with a 10 min scan sampling interval. The pen videos were observed continuously to count the total number of nest visits per nest and to calculate the duration of nest visits of five focal hens per pen.

    We found a relative preference for the small nest as more eggs, fewer nest visits per egg and longer nest visit durations were recorded for that nest. In addition, more hens – including more sitting hens – were in the small nests during the main egg-laying period, while the number of standing hens did not differ. These observations indicate that even though both nests may have been explored to a similar extent, the hens preferred the small nest for egg-laying.

    Learn more >>>

  • Beobachtung des Insektenflugs an&nbsp;Raps und Bienenweide

    Authors: Ziron, M.,&nbsp;Ziron, C., Glock, I.,&nbsp;Köpp, D. (2018)

    Abstract
    Im ersten Versuch zur Beobachtung des Insektenflugs wurde der Zuflug von Bienen und anderen Insekten im blühenden Raps ausgewertet. Versuch 2 beschäftigt sich dagegen mit dem Insektenflug an einer Bienenweidemischung. Raps gilt als besonders gute Bienenweidepflanze. Der Blühzeitraum beschränkt sich aber auf eine kurze intensive Periode im Frühjahr. Um das Trachtfließband über die gesamte Vegetationsperiode zu gewährleisten werden unterschiedliche Bienenweidemischungen angeboten. Untersucht wurde welche und wie viele Insekten die Kulturen anflogen. Außerdem wurde die Aufenthaltsdauer erfasst. Hierdurch ließen sich Schlüsse über die Attraktivität der jeweiligen Kultur für Bienen und andere Insekten ziehen. Die parallel erfassten Witterungsdaten zeigen, dass die Witterung ebenfalls einen großen Einfluss auf den Insektenflug hat.

    Poster vorgestellt beim Düsser Honigmarkt am 21.10.2018

    Download
  • The ontogeny of termite gathering among chimpanzees in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo

    Authors: Musgrave, S., Lonsdorf, E., Morgan, D., Sanz, C. (2020)&nbsp;

    Objectives
    Acquiring tool‐assisted foraging skills can potentially improve dietary quality and increase fitness for wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). In contrast to chimpanzees in East and West Africa, chimpanzees in the Congo Basin use tool sets and brush‐tipped fishing probes to gather termites. We investigated the ontogeny of these tool skills in chimpanzees of the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo, and compared it to that for chimpanzees at Gombe, Tanzania. We assessed whether chimpanzees acquired simple tool behaviors and single tool use before more complex actions and sequential use of multiple tool types.

    Materials and Methods
     
    Using a longitudinal approach, we scored remote video footage to document the acquisition of termite‐gathering critical elements for 25 immature chimpanzees at Goualougo.


    Results
     
    All chimpanzees termite fished by 2.9 years but did not manufacture brush‐tipped probes until an average of 4.3 years. Acquisition of sequential tool use extended into juvenility and adolescence. While we did not detect significant sex differences, most critical elements except tool manufacture were acquired slightly earlier by females.


    Discussion

    These findings contrast with Gombe, where chimpanzees learn to both use and make fishing probes between ages 1.5–3.5 and acquire the complete task by age 5.5. Differences between sites could reflect tool material selectivity and design complexity, the challenge of sequential tool behaviors, and strength requirements of puncturing subterranean termite nests at Goualougo. These results illustrate how task complexity may influence the timing and sequence of skill acquisition, improving models of the ontogeny of tool behavior among early hominins who likely used complex, perishable technologies.
     

    Learn more >>>

    Download
  • Insect prey characteristics affecting regional variation in chimpanzee tool use

    Authors: Sanz, C., Deblauwe, I., Tagg, N., Morgan, D. (2014)

    It is an ongoing interdisciplinary pursuit to identify the factors shaping the emergence and maintenance of tool technology. Field studies of several primate taxa have shown that tool using behaviors vary within and between populations. While similarity in tools over spatial and temporal scales may be the product of socially learned skills, it may also reflect adoption of convergent strategies that are tailored to specific prey features. Much has been claimed about regional variation in chimpanzee tool use, with little attention to the ecological circumstances that may have shaped such differences. This study examines chimpanzee tool use in termite gathering to evaluate the extent to which the behavior of insect prey may dictate chimpanzee technology. More specifically, we conducted a systematic comparison of chimpanzee tool use and termite prey between the Goualougo Triangle in the Republic of Congo and the La Belgique research site in southeast Cameroon. Apes at both of these sites are known to use tool sets to gather several species of termites. We collected insect specimens and measured the characteristics of their nests. Associated chimpanzee tool assemblages were documented at both sites and video recordings were conducted in the Goualougo Triangle. Although Macrotermitinae assemblages were identical, we found differences in the tools used to gather these termites. Based on measurements of the chimpanzee tools and termite nests at each site, we concluded that some characteristics of chimpanzee tools were directly related to termite nest structure. While there is a certain degree of uniformity within approaches to particular tool tasks across the species range, some aspects of regional variation in hominoid technology are likely adaptations to subtle environmental differences between populations or groups. Such microecological differences between sites do not negate the possibility of cultural transmission, as social learning may be required to transmit specific behaviors among individuals.

    Learn more>>>

  • Social niche shapes social behavior and cortisol concentrations during adolescence in female guinea pigs

    Authors: Rystrom T.L., Richter S.H., Sachser N., Kaiser S. (2024)

    Abstract: 

    Individualized social niches arise in social groups, resulting in divergent social behavior profiles among group members. During sensitive life phases, the individualized social niche can profoundly impact the development of social behavior and associated phenotypes such as hormone (e.g. cortisol) concentrations. Focusing on adolescence, we investigated the relationship between the individualized social niche, social behavior, and cortisol concentrations (baseline and responsiveness) in female guinea pigs. Females were pair-housed in early adolescence (initial social pair formation), and a social niche transition was induced after six weeks by replacing the partner with either a larger or smaller female. Regarding social behavior, dominance status was associated with aggression in both the initial social pairs and after the social niche transition, and the results suggest that aggression was rapidly and completely reshaped after the social niche transition. Meanwhile, submissive behavior was rapidly reshaped after the social niche transition, but this was incomplete. The dominance status attained in the initial social pair affected the extent of submissive behavior after the social niche transition, and this effect was still detected three weeks after the social niche transition. Regarding cortisol concentrations, higher baseline cortisol concentrations were measured in dominant females in the initial social pairs. After the social niche transition, cortisol responsiveness significantly increased for the females paired with a larger, older female relative to those paired with a smaller, younger female. These findings demonstrate that the social niche during adolescence plays a significant role in shaping behavior and hormone concentrations in females.

    Keywords: Social status; Individual variation; Cortisol reactivity; Phenotypic plasticity; Social environment; Individualized niche; Endocrine flexibility; Niche specialization; Stress response 

    Continue reading >>>

  • Studie: Pferdegerechte Einzäunung von Paddocks

    Authors: Mösenbacher-Molterer, I., Straub, I., Neumitka, D. &amp; Troxler, J.

    Als Aufwertung der reinen Boxenhaltung erfreut sich die Paddockhaltung unter Pferdebesitzern immer größerer Beliebtheit, zumal sie den Pferden durch Klimareize und eine freie Wahl des Aufenthaltsortes verbesserte Haltungsbedingungen verschafft. Eine aktuelle Studie des Lehr- und Forschungszentrums Raumberg-Gumpenstein sowie der Universität Wien untersucht die Pferdehaltung in stromführenden Paddocks hinsichtlich der Tiergerechtigkeit unter der Projektleitung von Ing. Irene Mösenbacher-Molterer. Auslöser des Projeks war eine Diskussion des österreichischen Tierschutzrates. Experten vermuteten, dass das Platzangebot eingeschränkt werden könnte, sobald das Pferd mit dem Strom in Berührung kommt - die Tiere befinden sich hier in einer Stresssituation und werden zukünftig vermutlich Sicherheitsabstand zur Umzäunung einhalten. Auch ein verminderter Sozialkontakt zum Nachbarpferd könnte daraus rückgeschlossen werden. Die Studie wurde im österreichischen Pferdezentrum Stadl Paura durchgeführt und sollte die möglichen Auswirkungen von stromführenden Zäunen auf das Pferd erfassen. Mittels Verhaltensbeobachtungen, Videoaufzeichnungen, der Aufzeichnung von Herzschlagfrequenz und Herzratenvariabilität wurden die physischen und psychischen Stressbelastungen sowie die effektive Ausnutzung der Fläche in stromführenden Kleinausläufen untersucht. In die Untersuchung wurden verschiedene Zaunsysteme und Pferdetypen einbezogen, um mittels einer großen Bandbreite ein aussagekräftiges Ergebnis zu erzielen. Für die Analyse des umfangreichen Datenmaterials wurde die Software INTERACT von Mangold International eingesetzt. Die Studie enthält abschließende Empfehlungen für die Paddockhaltung von Pferden hinsichtlich Größe, Abgrenzung und Tiergerechtigkeit, die auf der 2. Österreichischen Pferdefachtagung dem Fachpublikum vorgestellt wurden.

    Download
  • Untersuchung der lokomotorischen Aktivität von Schweinen im Verlauf der Mastperiode

    Authors: Julia Brendle, Steffen Hoy

    Ziel der Untersuchung war es, die innerhalb von 24h zurückgelegten Wegstrecken von Mastschweinen im Verlauf der Mastperiode zu erfassen und Aussagen über mögliche beeinflussende Faktoren zu treffen. Die Untersuchung erfolgte an insgesamt 220 Fokustieren, gehalten in 6er bzw. 12er Gruppen auf Vollspaltenboden. Wasser und die betriebseigene Futtermischung auf Getreidebasis für Anfangs- und Endmast standen während der Mastperiode ad libitum zur Verfügung. Zu Beginn der Mastperiode wurden alle Tiere gewogen und mit Hilfe einer 72h Videoaufzeichnung erfolgte die Ermittlung des individuellen Rangplatzes eines jeden Tieres in der Gruppe durch die Auswertung sämtlicher Rangkämpfe. Zu Mastbeginn, Mastmitte und Mastende (je 48h) wurden mit Hilfe der Software VideoMotionTracker (Mangold International) die während 24h zurückgelegten Wegstrecken der Fokustiere vermessen. Es zeigten sich signifikante Unterschiede zwischen den zurückgelegten Strecken innerhalb der Mastperiode (Mastbeginn = 730 m, Mastmitte = 501 m, Mastende = 315 m). Mastschweine in Gruppen mit 12 Tieren absolvierten während der gesamten Mastperiode längere Wegstrecken als Mastschweine in 6er Gruppen (z.B. Mastbeginn: 830 m versus 622 m). Die Differenzen zwischen den Mittelwerten waren dabei höchstsignifikant (p < 0,001). Tendenziell legten weibliche Tiere im Mastverlauf durchschnittlich längere Wegstrecken zurück, wohingegen die Rangklasse der Mastschweine keinen signifikanten Einfluss auf die Wegstreckenlänge der einzelnen Fokustiere ausübte. Der Parameter Lebendmasse war im gesamten Mastverlauf negativ mit dem Parameter Wegstrecke korreliert.

    Download
  • Tool Use in Animals

    Authors: C. Sanz, J. Call, C. Boesch

    Kapuzineraffen, die mit Steinen Nüsse knacken oder Schimpansen, die mit Stöcken Termiten angeln. Sie sind Beispiele für Tiere, die erfinderisch Werkzeuge einsetzen - meist, um sich Nahrungsquellen zu erschließen. Einen interdisziplinären Einblick in das Thema Werkzeuggebrauch bei Tieren bietet das im Cambridge Verlag erschienene Buch "Tool Use in Animals" (nur in englischer Sprache erhältlich).   

    Das Buch untersucht die kognitiven Fähigkeiten und umweltbedingten Faktoren, die die Werkzeugentwicklung und -nutzung bei Tieren geprägt haben. Das Besondere: Die Tiere wurden nicht in Laborstudien beobachtet, sondern in freier Wildbahn. Damit ist die Publikation eine spannende Lektüre und richtet sich nicht nur an wissenschaftliches Publikum.   

    "Tool Use in Animals" stellt Beiträge verschiedener führender Autoren aus Psychologie, Biologie und Anthropologie vor. Die Herausgeber sind Dr. Crickette Sanz, Anthropologin an der Washington University, Dr. Josep Call, Psychologe am Max-Planck-Institut sowie Prof. Christophe Boesch, Direktor des Max-Planck-Instituts für Evolutionäre Anthropologie.   

    Dr. Crickette Sanz ist zudem Mitglied des Mangold Scientific Councils und engagiert im Goualougo Triangle Ape Project, das sich der Erforschung und Bewahrung von Großaffen im Kongo verschrieben hat.

  • Contextualised behavioural measurements of personality differences obtained in behavioural tests and social observations in adult capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella)

    Authors: Jana Uher, Elsa Addessi, Elisabetta Visalberghi

    We applied a new framework for behavioural research on personality differences in 26 adult tufted capuchin monkeys. Using the Behavioural Repertoire x Environmental Situations Approach, we generated systematically 20 non-lexical emic personality constructs that have high ecological validity for this species. For construct operationalisation, we obtained 146 contextualised behavioural measures repeatedly in 15 experimental situations and 2 group situations using computerised and video-assisted methods. A complete repetition after a 2–3-week break within a 60-day period yielded significant test–retest reliability from individual-oriented and variable-oriented viewpoints at different levels of aggregation. In accordance with well-established findings on cross-situational consistency, internal consistency was only moderate. This new and important finding highlights fundamental differences between behavioural approaches and judgment-based approaches to personality differences.

    Link to "Journal of Research in Personality" >>>

  • Aping expressions: Chimpanzees produce distinct laugh types when responding to laughter of others

    Authors: M. Davila Ross, B. Allcock, C. Thomas, K.A. Bard

    Humans have the ability to replicate the emotional expressions of others even when they undergo different emotions. Such distinct responses of expressions, especially positive expressions, play a central role in everyday social communication of humans and may give the responding individuals important advantages in cooperation and communication.

    Download
  • A dual function of echolocation: bats use echolocation calls to identify familiar and unfamiliar individuals

    Authors: Silke L. Voigt-Heucke, Michael Taborsky, Dina K.N. Dechmann

    Bats use echolocation for orientation during foraging and navigation. However, it has been suggested that echolocation calls may also have a communicative function, for instance between roost members. In principle, this seems possible because echolocation calls are species specific and known to differ between the sexes, and between colonies and individuals for some species. We performed playback experiments with lesser bulldog bats, Noctilio albiventris, to which we presented calls of familiar/unfamiliar conspecifics, cohabitant/noncohabitant heterospecifics and ultrasonic white noise as a control. Bats reacted with a complex repertoire of social behaviours and the intensity of their response differed significantly between stimulus categories. Stronger reactions were shown towards echolocation calls of unfamiliar conspecifics than towards heterospecifics and white noise. To our knowledge, this is the first time that bats have been found to react to echolocation calls with a suite of social behaviours. Our results also provide the first experimental evidence for acoustical differentiation by bats between familiar and unfamiliar conspecifics, and of heterospecifics. Analysis of echolocation calls confirmed significant individual differences between echolocation calls. In addition, we found a nonsignificant trend towards group signatures in echolocation calls of N. albiventris. We suggest that echolocation calls used during orientation may also communicate species identity, group affiliation and individual identity. Our study highlights the communicative potential of sonar signals that have previously been categorized as cues in animal social systems.  

  • Genetic Architecture of Tameness in a Rat Model of Animal Domestication

    Authors: Frank W. Albert

    Abstract of the main publication: A common feature of domestic animals is tameness—i.e., they tolerate and are unafraid of human presence and handling. To gain insight into the genetic basis of tameness and aggression, we studied an intercross between two lines of rats (Rattus norvegicus) selected over.60 generations for increased tameness and increased aggression against humans, respectively. We measured 45 traits, including tameness and aggression, anxiety-related traits, organ weights, and levels of serum components in .700 rats from an intercross population. Using 201 genetic markers, we identified two significant quantitative trait loci (QTL) for tameness. These loci overlap with QTL for adrenal gland weight and for anxiety-related traits and are part of a five-locus epistatic network influencing tameness. An additional QTL influences the occurrence of white coat spots, but shows no significant effect on tameness. The loci described here are important starting points for finding the genes that cause tameness in these rats and potentially in domestic animals in general.

    Link to Genetics website >>>

  • Rapid facial mimicry in orangutan play

    Authors: Marina Davila Ross, Susanne Menzler and Elke Zimmermann

    Abstract
    Emotional contagion enables individuals to experience emotions of others. This important empathic phenomenon is closely linked to facial mimicry, where facial displays evoke the same facial expressions in social partners. In humans, facial mimicry can be voluntary or involuntary, whereby its latter mode can be processed as rapid as within or at 1 s. Thus far, studies have not provided evidence of rapid involuntary facial mimicry in animals.

    This study assessed whether rapid involuntary facial mimicry is present in orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus; NZ25) for their open-mouth faces (OMFs) during everyday dyadic play. Results clearly indicated that orangutans rapidly mimicked OMFs of their playmates within or at 1 s. Our study revealed the first evidence on rapid involuntary facial mimicry in non-human mammals. This finding suggests that fundamental building blocks of positive emotional contagion and empathy that link to rapid involuntary facial mimicry in humans have homologues in non-human primates.

    Download
  • Responding to inequities: gorillas try to maintain their competitive advantage during play fights

    Authors: Edwin Van Leeuwen, Elke Zimmermann and Marina Davila Ross

    Abstract: Humans respond to unfair situations in various ways. Experimental research has revealed that non-human species also respond to unequal situations in the formof inequity aversions when they have the disadvantage. The current study focused on play fights in gorillas to explore for the first time, to our knowledge, if/how non-human species respond to inequities in natural social settings. Hitting causes a naturally occurring inequity among individuals and here it was specifically assessed how the hitters and their partners engaged in play chases that followed the hitting. The results of this work showed that the hitters significantly more often moved first to run away immediately after the encounter than their partners. These findings provide evidence that non-human species respond to inequities by trying to maintain their competitive advantages. We conclude that non-human primates, like humans, may show different responses to inequities and that they may modify them depending on if they have the advantage or the disadvantage.

    Download
  • Erdrückungsursachen in Abferkelbuchten ohne Fixierung der Sau

    Authors: Nora Warter

    Präsentation im Rahmen der Öko-Schweinehalter-Tagung 2010.
    Wo, wann, wie und weshalb werden die Ferkel in den Abferkelbuchten (LSZ Boxberg) ohne Fixierung der Sau erdrückt?
    Bauliche Maßnahmen zur Verbesserung der Situation.

    Download
  • Neue Methoden der Aktivitätsregistrierung (Nicht-invasive Verhaltensmessungen bei Paarhufern)

    Authors: Peer Cyriacks, Siegfried Rieger

    Abstrakt:
    Seit einigen Jahren ist es der Wildtierforschung möglich, Aktivitätsdaten von Wildtieren mittels Telemetriehalsbändern zu gewinnen. Dennoch kommt dieser Technik bisher meist eine geringe Bedeutung zu und nur wenige Studien konzentrieren sich auf eine wirkungsvolle Interpretation dieser Daten.

    Download
  • Kin recognition in the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus)

    Authors: Institute of Zoology, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover / Lab course: Experimental Behavioural Biology

    Abstract
    Kin recognition is a prerequisite for kin selection. Kin selection has been theorized as a driving force behind the evolution of group-living in primates. vocal recognition of kin has been observed in haplorhine primates (Rendall, 2004) and in the diurnal, gregarious strepsirrhine, Lemur catta (Nunn, 2000). Much less research has been done on the vocalizations of the nocturnal, solitarily foraging strepsirrhines. Our study is the first to test for vocal recognition of kin in a nongregarious strepsirrhine. Mouse lemurs are small-boiled, nocturnal, solitarily foraging strepsirrhine primates that have dispersed social networks (Braune et al., 2008). We have testet whether M. murinus females respond differently to and whistles, an alarm call (Braune et al., 2008), and trills, advertisement calls, given by their father and by unrelated males.

    Download
  • Food preference in two mouse lemur species (Microcebus lehilahytsara &amp; Microcebus murinus)

    Authors: Institute of Zoology, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover / Lab course: Experimental Behavioural Biology

    Abstract
    Two different species of mouse lemurs (Microcebus lehilahytsara and M. murinus) were tested for their food preferences. Four different food items were presented in a two paired choice test to find the most adequate reward for upcoming behavioural tests.

    Download
  • Acoustic cues of caller identity and affect intensity in communication calls of tree shrews (Tupaia belangeri)

    Authors: Institute of Zoology, University of Veterinary Medicine Hannover / Lab course: Experimental Behavioural Biology

    Abstract
    Comparative studies on the vocalisation of humans and animals have shown that structural and temporal variations in communication sounds serve several functions, such as to reliably transmit the affective state and individuality of the sender. These variations within a call type are named acoustic cues and are ghoutght to be important factors in the communication process of social living animals. In the present study, we have examined attention calls of tree shrews (Tupaia belangeri) for acoustic cues conveying the affective state and/or individuality of the sender. Any general physiological activation of the nervous system in a tree shrew leads to defined changes in its behavioural patterns. When aroused, it raises its tail/ruffles its tail hair and sometimes utters attention calls (von Holst, 1977). Tree shrews utter these calls in their natural habitat, when they are confronted with new environmental stimuli (Emmons, 2000).

    Download
  • Personality in the behaviour of great apes - temporal stability, cross-situational consistency

    Authors: Jana Uher, Jens B. Asendorpf and Josep Call

    Abstract
    Using a multidisciplinary approach, the present study complements ethological behaviour measurements with basic theoretical concepts, methods and approaches of the personality psychological trait paradigm. Its adoptability and usefulness for animal studies is tested exemplarily on a sample of 20 zoo-housed great apes (five of each of the following species): bonobos, Pan paniscus; chimpanzees, Pan troglodytes verus; gorillas, Gorilla gorilla gorilla; and orang-utans, Pongo pygmaeus abelii. Data on 76 single trait-relevant behaviours were recorded in a series of 14 laboratory based situations and in two different group situations. Data collection was repeated completely after a break of two weeks within a 50-day period. All behaviour records were sufficiently reliable. Individual- and variable-oriented analyses showed high/substantial temporal stability on different levels of aggregation. Distinctive and stable individual situational and response profiles clarified the importance of situations and of multiple trait-relevant behaviours. The present study calls for a closer collaboration between personality psychologists and behavioural biologists to tap the full potential of animal personality research.

    Download
  • Personality assessment in the Great Apes: Comparing ecologically valid behavior measures, behavior ratings, and adjective ratings

    Authors: Jana Uher and Jens B. Asendorpf

    Abstract
    Three methods of personality assessment (behavior measures, behavior ratings, adjective ratings) were compared in 20 zoo-housed Great Apes: bonobos (Pan paniscus), chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes verus), gorillas (Gorilla gorilla gorilla), and orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii). To test a new bottom-up approach, the studied trait constructs were systematically generated from the species’ behavioral repertoires. The assessments were reliable, temporally stable, and showed substantial cross-method coherence. In most traits, behavior ratings mediated the relations between adjective ratings and behavior measures. Results suggest that high predictability of manifest behavior is best achieved by behavior ratings, not by adjectives. Empirical evidence for trait constructs beyond current personality models points to the necessity of broad and systematic approaches for valid inferences on a species’ personality structure.

    Download
  • Cooperative Activities in Young Children and Chimpanzees

    Authors: Felix Warneken, Frances Chen and Michael Tomasello

    Abstract
    Human children 18 – 24 months of age and 3 young chimpanzees interacted in 4 cooperative activities with a human adult partner. The human children successfully participated in cooperative problem-solving activities and social games, whereas the chimpanzees were uninterested in the social games. As an experimental manipulation, in each task the adult partner stopped participating at a specific point during the activity. All children produced at least one communicative attempt to reengage him, perhaps suggesting that they were trying to reinstate a shared goal. No chimpanzee ever made any communicative attempt to reengage the partner. These results are interpreted as evidence for a uniquely human form of cooperative activity involving shared intentionality that emerges in the second year of life.

    Download
  • Interaktionsmuster und nonverbale Kommunikation psychisch auffälliger Kinder mit einem Therapiehund

    Authors: Prothmann A., Albrecht K., Dietrich S., Hornfeck U., Stieber S. und Ettrich C.

    Kommunikation und Interaktion spielen bei psychischen Störungen eine zentrale Rolle. Dabei sind nonverbale Interaktionsmerkmale so bedeutsam wie Sprachzeichen, unterliegen aber bedeutend weniger manipulativen Einflüssen als verbale Kommunikation. Da Kinder eine starke natürliche Affinität zu Tieren haben und mit diesen überwiegend nonverbal über Mimik, Gestik, Körperhaltung und Stimmmodulation interagieren, bietet sich die Möglichkeit, den Einfluss psychischer Störungen auf die nonverbalen Kommunikationsfertigkeiten direkt zu beobachten.

    Download
  • Auswirkungen verschiedener Kraftfuttervorlagetechniken bei Pferden in Einzelboxenhaltung

    Authors: Torsten Hohmann, Peter Kreimeier, Franz-Josef Bockisch und Willa Bohnet

    Bei der Haltung von Pferden in Einzelboxen besteht bei manueller Kraftfuttervorlage häufig das Problem, dass die Pferde unruhig werden, Verletzungen und Stress ausgesetzt sind, da das Futter nicht zeitgleich verabreicht wird. Es wird eine Verhaltensanalyse mit einer Analyse der Herzfrequenzvariabilität verknüpft, um eine mögliche Stressbelastung während der Kraftfuttervorlage anhand verschiedener Futterapplikationstechniken bewerten zu können.

    Download
  • Bewegungsaktivität und Stressbelastung bei Pferden in Auslaufhaltungssystemen mit verschiedenen Haltungsformen&nbsp;

    Authors: Dr. med. vet. Gundula Hoffmann

    Abstrakt:
    Die Haltung von Pferden als Freizeitpartner des Menschen erfreut sich großer Beliebtheit, erfüllt jedoch nicht immer die natürlichen Bedürfnisse der Pferde, was sich in einem gehäuften Auftreten haltungsbedingter Erkrankungen widerspiegelt. Noch immer stellt die Unterbringung in Einzelboxen die häufigste Pferdehaltungsform dar, wobei sie nur einen eingeschränkten Sozialkontakt zu Artgenossen und wenig Wahlmöglichkeiten in Bezug auf den Aufenthaltsort bieten kann. Hinzu kommt, dass ein Reitpferd durchschnittlich eine Stunde pro Tag bewegt wird und nicht selten bis zu 23 Stunden täglich in seiner Unterkunft verbringt, sodass der Optimierung der Pferdehaltungssysteme eine größere Aufmerksamkeit geschenkt werden sollte.

    Eine entscheidende Verbesserung und Anpassung an das natürliche Verhalten der Pferde wurde bereits durch die Gruppenhaltung in Mehrraum-Auslaufhaltungssystemen erreicht, welche neben der Weidehaltung als eine der artgerechtesten Haltungsformen gilt (HENNING 2004). Das Pferd befindet sich in Herdengesellschaft und kann sich im Auslauf frei bewegen. Die Gruppenauslaufhaltung trägt daher dem grundlegenden Wandel der Pferdenutzung vom regelmäßig genutzten Arbeitstier zum meist nur noch unregelmäßig beanspruchten Freizeitbegleiter am konsequentesten Rechnung (PIOTROWSKI u. KREIMEIER 1998). Untersuchungen am ehemaligen Institut für landwirtschaftliche Bauforschung (FAL Braunschweig) mit Haflingern in solch einer Haltungsform konnten bereits aufzeigen, dass die Fütterungsfrequenz und die Weglänge zu den Fressplätzen einen höheren Einfluss auf die zurückgelegten Wegstrecken innerhalb des Haltungssystems haben, als die Möglichkeit der freien Bewegung im Haltungssystem (FRENTZEN 1994).

    Unter welchen Bedingungen die Haltung von Pferden in Auslaufhaltungen allerdings dem Bewegungsbedürfnis der Pferde gerecht wird und in welcher Form das tägliche Stress- und Bewegungsverhalten der Pferde durch zusätzliche Bewegungsangebote beeinflusst wird, scheint indes noch nicht ausreichend untersucht.

    Link zur Studie >>>

  • Computer Supported Measurement of Distance Moved by Rabbits a day by Mangold Video Motion Tracker

    Authors: Steffen Hoy, Justus Liebig University of Gießen

    Abstract
    Because of several reasons it was necessary to develop and to test a new software solution to analyze the distance moved by farm animals in the field.

    Download
  • Teaching varies with task complexity in wild chimpanzees

    Authors: Stephanie Musgravea, Elizabeth Lonsdorf, David Morgan, Madison Prestipino, Laura Bernstein-Kurtycze, Roger Mundry, and Crickette SanzEnter author(s)

    Cumulative culture is a transformative force in human evolution, but the social underpinnings of this capacity are debated. Identi-fying social influences on how chimpanzees acquire tool tasks of differing complexity may help illuminate the evolutionary origins of technology in our own lineage. Humans routinely transfer tools to novices to scaffold their skill development. While tool transfers occur in wild chimpanzees and fulfill criteria for teaching, it is unknown whether this form of helping varies between populations and across tasks. Applying standardized methods, we compared tool transfers during termite gathering by chimpanzees in the Goualougo Triangle, Republic of Congo, and in Gombe, Tanzania. At Goualougo, chimpanzees use multiple, different tool types sequentially, choose specific raw materials, and perform modifications that improve tool efficiency, which could make it challenging for novices to manufac-ture suitable tools. Termite gathering at Gombe involves a single tool type, fishing probes, which can be manufactured from various materials. Multiple measures indicated population differences in tool-transfer behavior. The rate of transfers and probability of transfer upon request were significantly higher at Goualougo, while resistance to transfers was significantly higher at Gombe. Active transfers of tools in which possessors moved to facilitate possession change upon request occurred only at Goualougo, where they were the most common transfer type. At Gombe, tool requests were typically refused. We suggest that these population differences in tool-transfer behavior may relate to task complexity and that active helping plays an enhanced role in the cultural transmission of complextechnology in wild apes.

    Download