Product · 19 min read
What is Unconscious, Non-Conscious, and Pre-conscious?
A brief review of the unconscious, non-conscious, and pre-conscious processes that influence human thinking and actions.

The Architecture of Awareness: Differentiating Unconscious, Non-Conscious, and Pre-Conscious Mental Processes
Abstract
This article provides a comprehensive overview of the distinctions between unconscious, non-conscious and pre-conscious mental processes — critical concepts in cognitive science, psychology and neuroscience. Although these terms are often conflated in popular discourse, a precise scientific understanding is essential for the accurate theoretical modelling and empirical investigation of the human mind. We explore the historical context, focusing on the Freudian psychoanalytic framework, and compare it with modern neuroscientific and cognitive psychological perspectives. Key phenomena such as implicit processes, subliminal perception, blindsight, predictive coding and autonomic regulation are examined as manifestations of these distinct levels of awareness. We discuss methodological approaches, including neuroimaging (EEG, ERP, fMRI) and behavioural paradigms (masked prime tasks and reaction time experiments), as tools for their empirical investigation. The article concludes by emphasising the profound implications of these non-conscious and unconscious processes for emotional regulation, decision-making and overall human behaviour.
1. Introduction: The Levels of Mental Operation
Human cognition and behaviour are the result of an intricate interplay of processes operating at various levels of awareness. Although conscious experience represents our immediate subjective reality, a significant amount of mental activity occurs beyond the scope of direct introspection. The terms ‘unconscious’, ‘non-conscious’ and ‘subconscious’ are often used to describe these hidden mental processes, but their precise definitions and scientific usefulness can differ greatly from how they are used in everyday speech.
The exploration of non-conscious mental states gained prominence historically with Sigmund Freud’s psychoanalytic theory, which posited an active unconscious mind filled with repressed desires and memories. However, modern cognitive science and neuroscience have expanded and refined these concepts, identifying distinct categories of non-conscious processing that can be verified empirically and explained mechanistically. Understanding these distinctions is crucial for advancing research in cognitive psychology, behavioural neuroscience and clinical practice, as these underlying processes have a profound influence on perception, emotion, decision-making and motor control.
This review will systematically define and differentiate between the conscious, unconscious, non-conscious and pre-conscious domains, providing empirical examples and relevant theoretical models to illustrate each one. We will also discuss the problematic nature of the term ‘subconscious’ in scientific discourse, outlining the methodologies used to study these elusive mental phenomena.

2. The Conscious Mind: Phenomenal Awareness and Executive Control
Consciousness is often defined as the state of being aware of one’s internal and external existence. It represents the pinnacle of subjective experience and cognitive control. It encompasses phenomenal awareness of sensory inputs, thoughts, emotions and memories, as well as the capacity for deliberate introspection and volitional action. The conscious mind is characterised by its limited capacity and sequential processing, as well as its role in novel problem solving, planning and flexible behaviour (Baars, 1988).
Key attributes of conscious processing include:
- Subjective experience (qualia): The unique, subjective qualitative nature of sensations, perceptions and emotions.
- Attention and working memory: The ability to focus selectively on relevant information and keep it in an active, easily accessible state for manipulation.
- Volitional control: The capacity to intentionally initiate or inhibit actions guided by goals and plans.
- Metacognition: The awareness and regulation of one’s own cognitive processes, including the monitoring and evaluation of one’s thoughts and learning.
Although the exact neural correlates of consciousness are still being researched, it is widely accepted that they involve widespread, integrated activity across various brain regions, particularly the prefrontal and parietal cortices and the thalamus (Dehaene & Changeux, 2011). Conscious awareness acts as a kind of global broadcasting system, making information available to many specialised cognitive modules and facilitating complex decision-making and adaptive responses to environmental demands (Baars, 2002).
3. The Unconscious: Dynamic Influences Beyond Direct Access
The concept of the unconscious mind has evolved significantly since its initial formulation. In contemporary scientific psychology, the unconscious refers to mental processes and contents that are not currently accessible to conscious awareness but can nonetheless influence thoughts, feelings and behaviours. This differs from the Freudian psychoanalytic concept of the unconscious, which emphasises repressed material, often of a traumatic nature, that is actively held out of awareness by psychological defence mechanisms (Freud, 1915).

3.1. Freudian Psychoanalytic Unconscious
Sigmund Freud proposed that the unconscious mind was the primary determinant of personality and psychopathology. This dynamic unconscious was conceived as a repository for unacceptable thoughts, memories and desires, particularly those related to early childhood experiences which were repressed because they were perceived as threatening. According to Freud, these unconscious conflicts manifest themselves indirectly through dreams (latent content), neurotic symptoms and parapraxes (e.g. Freudian slips). Psychoanalytic techniques such as free association and dream analysis were developed to reveal these hidden contents and resolve underlying psychological distress (Freud, 1915). Although there is limited empirical support for many Freudian constructs, his emphasis on mental processes operating outside of awareness laid the groundwork for subsequent research.
3.2. Modern Cognitive Unconscious
Contemporary cognitive psychology and neuroscience conceptualise the unconscious as a collection of diverse processes that are often adaptive and operate automatically and efficiently without requiring conscious oversight. These processes are not necessarily repressed, they simply lie outside the current focus of attention. Key manifestations include:
- Implicit processes: These refer to cognitive operations that occur without conscious intention or awareness yet still impact behaviour. Examples include:
- Implicit memory: Knowledge acquired and expressed without conscious recollection, such as procedural skills (e.g. riding a bicycle or typing), or the effects of past experiences on current performance (e.g. priming) (Schacter, 1987).
- Implicit learning: The acquisition of complex information without being aware of what has been learned or how the learning occurred (Reber, 1989).
- Subliminal stimuli and perception: Stimuli that are presented below the threshold of conscious perception (e.g. very brief visual presentations or faint auditory cues) can still be processed by the brain and can influence subsequent judgements, attitudes or behaviours (Kouider & Dehaene, 2007). Neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that subliminal stimuli can activate relevant brain regions even in the absence of a conscious report (Whalen et al., 1998).
- Priming: A phenomenon in which exposure to one stimulus (the prime) influences the processing of a subsequent stimulus (the target), often without the individual being aware of the prime. Priming effects can be semantic, perceptual or conceptual, demonstrating the interconnectedness of mental representations and the automatic activation of associated information (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999).
These modern perspectives emphasise the functional and adaptive nature of unconscious processing, showing how it influences everyday cognition, including perception, memory, decision-making and social behaviour.

4. The Non-Conscious: Inaccessible Physiological and Cognitive Operations
The term ‘non-conscious’ refers to mental and physiological processes that operate entirely outside the realm of conscious awareness and are inherently inaccessible to introspection. Unlike the unconscious, which may contain content that could, in principle, be brought to consciousness (e.g. through therapy or heightened attention), non-conscious processes are fundamental, automatic operations not designed for conscious access. These processes are essential for maintaining bodily homeostasis, executing routine actions and interacting rapidly with the environment.
Examples of non-conscious processes include:
- Autonomic regulation: The involuntary control of vital bodily functions, such as heart rate, respiration, digestion and thermoregulation, by the autonomic nervous system (ANS). These processes operate continuously and automatically to maintain physiological equilibrium without requiring conscious intervention (Harrison et al., 2013).
- Motor Preparation and Execution: The complex neural computations involved in planning, initiating and executing movements often occur outside of conscious awareness. Although the intention to move may be conscious, the intricate sequence of muscle activations and sensory feedback loops are largely unconscious. Studies using electrophysiology (e.g. readiness potentials) demonstrate preparatory brain activity that precedes the conscious awareness of an impending movement (Libet et al., 1983).
- Blindsight: This is a remarkable neurological condition resulting from damage to the primary visual cortex (V1). Patients with this condition report no conscious visual experience in a specific part of their visual field, yet they can still respond accurately to visual stimuli presented within that ‘blind’ field. They might, for example, correctly guess the orientation of a line or the direction of movement, demonstrating that visual information is processed non-consciously via alternative subcortical pathways (Weiskrantz, 1986). Blindsight provides compelling evidence for a dissociation between visual processing and conscious perception.
- Visual neglect (hemispatial neglect): A neuropsychological disorder typically resulting from damage of one hemisphere of the brain, in which patients fail to attend to or respond to stimuli presented on the opposite side of space, despite having intact sensory and motor capabilities. Crucially, patients are often unaware of their neglect, highlighting the non-conscious nature of the attentional deficit (Vallar & Perani, 1986). This condition highlights how fundamental aspects of spatial awareness can operate outside of conscious experience.
These non-conscious processes are essential for efficient and adaptive functioning. They offload routine and vital operations from the limited capacity of conscious awareness. This enables the conscious mind to focus on novel or complex challenges.
5. The Pre-Conscious: Information on the Threshold of Awareness
The pre-conscious mind is a distinct level of mental functioning characterised by information that is not currently in conscious awareness, but which is readily accessible and can be retrieved with minimal effort. It acts as a mental buffer or temporary storage area for information that is not currently in use, but can be recalled immediately. Initially described by Freud as a ‘mental waiting room’ where thoughts reside before entering consciousness (Freud, 1915), the pre-conscious mind is a distinct level of mental operation.
Examples of pre-conscious content include:
-
Accessible memories: Personal facts (e.g. one’s address or phone number), general knowledge (e.g. the capital of France) and recent events (e.g. what one had for breakfast) are usually stored in the subconscious mind. These memories do not occupy constant conscious thought, but can be easily retrieved when prompted.
-
Skills and habits (when not actively engaged): While the execution of highly practised skills can be unconscious (e.g. walking), the knowledge of how to perform them resides in the subconscious when not actively engaged and can be brought to conscious reflection if needed (e.g. explaining how to tie a shoelace).
The preconscious mind acts as a crucial intermediary, facilitating the flow of information between the vast and inaccessible unconscious mind and the limited active conscious mind. It enables efficient cognitive functioning by providing relevant information without overwhelming conscious processing.
6. The Subconscious: A Term Lacking Scientific Precision
The term ‘subconscious’ is widely used in popular psychology, self-help literature and everyday language, often interchangeably with ‘unconscious’ or ‘preconscious’. However, it is largely avoided within academic psychology and neuroscience due to its ambiguity and lack of precise scientific definition. Sigmund Freud initially used the term ‘subconscious’, but later abandoned it in favour of the more rigorously defined ‘unconscious’ to denote the dynamic, repressed aspects of the psyche (Freud, 1915).
The primary issue with ‘subconscious’ is its imprecision. Phenomena often attributed to the ‘subconscious’ are more accurately categorised under scientifically defined concepts such as:
- Pre-conscious: Information that is readily accessible but not currently in awareness.
- Implicit processes: Automatic, non-conscious cognitive operations that influence behaviour without explicit awareness.
Although the term has popular appeal, its continued use in scientific discourse can lead to conceptual confusion and hinder clear communication about the different levels of mental processing. Researchers therefore advocate using “unconscious”, “non-conscious” and “pre-conscious” to maintain terminological rigour and facilitate empirical investigation.
INTERACT: One Software for Your Entire Observational Research Workflow
From audio/video-based content-coding and transcription to analysis - INTERACT has you covered.

7. Methodological Approaches to Investigating Non-Conscious and Unconscious Processes
Investigating mental processes that operate outside of conscious awareness presents unique methodological challenges. To infer the existence and mechanisms of these elusive phenomena, researchers employ a combination of neuroscientific and behavioural techniques.
7.1. Neuroimaging Techniques
Neuroimaging provides invaluable tools for observing brain activity associated with non-conscious and unconscious processing.
- Electroencephalography (EEG): This measures electrical activity generated by neuronal populations and offers high temporal resolution. It is particularly useful for identifying rapid, transient brain responses to stimuli that may not reach conscious awareness, such as event-related potentials (ERPs) (Luck, 2014). ERP components (e.g. N2pc and P300) can differentiate between the conscious and unconscious processing of visual or auditory stimuli.
- Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI): It measures changes in blood oxygenation levels (the BOLD signal), which are indicative of neural activity. This provides high spatial resolution and allows researchers to identify the specific brain regions involved in unconscious processes. For example, the amygdala can be activated in response to subliminally presented fearful faces, even when the subject reports no conscious perception of the face (Whalen et al., 1998). It can also delineate the neural pathways involved in blindsight, demonstrating activity in subcortical visual areas despite damage to V1 (Weiskrantz, 1986).
7.2. Behavioral Paradigms
Behavioural experiments are designed to infer unconscious processing by demonstrating its influence on observable responses.
- Masked-prime tasks: In these tasks, a prime stimulus is presented very briefly (e.g. 10–50 ms) and is immediately followed by a masking stimulus, which makes the prime invisible to the conscious mind. Evidence for unconscious processing is provided by the effect of the masked prime on the processing of a subsequent target stimulus (e.g. faster reaction times for congruent primes) (Dehaene et al., 1998).
- Reaction time experiments: Differences in reaction times to various stimuli can reveal the influence of non-conscious processes. For instance, in tasks involving motor preparation, preparatory neural activity (e.g. readiness potential) can be observed prior to conscious awareness of the intention to move, thereby influencing response speed (Libet et al., 1983).
7.3. Clinical and Psychometric Approaches
Although they are less direct and often more controversial, certain clinical and psychometric methods have been used to explore unconscious content.
- Free association and dream analysis: Rooted in the psychoanalytic tradition, these methods aim to reveal unconscious thoughts and conflicts by analysing spontaneous verbalisations and dream content. Although their scientific validity as empirical tools for direct access to the unconscious is debated, they remain central to psychodynamic therapies (Freud, 1915).
- Hypnosis paradigms: Hypnosis has been explored as a means of accessing repressed memories or influencing behaviour outside of one’s conscious will. However, research indicates that memories retrieved under hypnosis are highly susceptible to suggestion and confabulation, raising concerns about their accuracy (Loftus & Ketcham, 1994).
- Self-report measures: Although they primarily assess conscious experiences, self-report measures can sometimes provide indirect insights into the effects of unconscious processes (e.g. changes in mood or preferences without a clear conscious reason). However, by definition, their utility for directly probing unconscious content is inherently limited.
These diverse methodologies, spanning neurophysiology, experimental psychology and clinical practice, collectively contribute to our evolving understanding of the complex interplay between conscious and unconscious mental processes.
8. Theoretical Frameworks for Understanding Awareness
Several overarching theoretical frameworks attempt to explain the mechanisms underlying conscious and non-conscious processing, beyond specific phenomena.
8.1. Global Workspace Theory (GWT)
Proposed by Bernard Baars in 1988, Global Workspace Theory (GWT) is a well-known cognitive model used to explain consciousness. GWT posits a ‘global workspace’ (analogous to a theatre stage) where information from various specialised unconscious processors (the ‘audience’ or ‘backstage crew’) can be broadcast to the entire system. Once information gains access to the global workspace, it becomes available to the conscious mind and can be accessed by other specialised modules, facilitating widespread integration and flexible, adaptive behaviour. Information that does not gain access to the global workspace remains unconscious or non-conscious. GWT provides a compelling model of how conscious experience emerges from a vast network of unconscious computations, emphasising the importance of sharing information globally within the brain (Baars, 2005).
8.2. Predictive Coding and the Bayesian Brain
Predictive coding is an influential theory in neuroscience and cognitive science which posits that the brain is a hierarchical inference machine that is constantly generating and updating predictions about sensory input (Friston, 2005). According to this framework, the brain actively predicts what it expects to perceive and compares these predictions with actual sensory information. Any discrepancy between the prediction and the input generates a ‘prediction error’, which is then propagated up the hierarchy to update the internal model. Much of this predictive processing occurs unconsciously, with conscious perception arising from the minimisation of prediction errors and the refinement of the internal model (Clark, 2013). Predictive coding provides a unifying framework through which various cognitive phenomena, including perception, attention and learning, can be understood, and highlights how unconscious expectations can profoundly shape our conscious experience and behaviour.

9. Implications for Emotional Processing and Decision-Making
The intricate interplay between conscious, unconscious and non-conscious processes has profound implications for our understanding of how humans process emotions and make decisions. These hidden layers of the mind are active determinants of our emotional states and behavioural choices, rather than passive repositories.
9.1. Emotional Processing
Emotional responses are often triggered and influenced by unconscious processes. For example, research shows that presenting emotionally salient stimuli (e.g. fearful faces) subliminally can activate the amygdala and cause physiological arousal (e.g. skin conductance responses), even when people report no awareness of the stimuli (Whalen et al., 1998). This suggests that rapid subcortical pathways can process emotional information pre-consciously, leading to affective responses that either precede or bypass conscious appraisal. Furthermore, implicit emotional learning, such as the classical conditioning of fear responses, can occur without recollection of the learning event and influence subsequent emotional reactions, contributing to conditions such as anxiety disorders (LeDoux, 1996). Therefore, understanding these unconscious emotional drivers is crucial for developing effective therapeutic interventions.
9.2. Decision-Making
Although conscious deliberation is commonly regarded as the main driver of decision-making, a substantial body of evidence suggests that unconscious processes also have a significant impact. As discussed, priming effects can subtly bias judgements and choices without the decision-maker’s awareness (Bargh & Chartrand, 1999). Heuristics and biases are often automatic, unconscious cognitive shortcuts that can lead to systematic deviations from rational decision-making (Kahneman, 2011).
Furthermore, the brain’s predictive coding mechanisms generate continuous expectations that guide our actions and choices, often operating outside of our conscious awareness. This enables efficient and rapid decision-making in familiar contexts while conserving conscious resources for novel or complex situations. The non-conscious regulation of physiological states via the autonomic nervous system impacts cognitive function and emotional states, thereby indirectly influencing decision outcomes. For instance, physiological arousal associated with stress can impair executive functions and alter risk assessment (Arnsten, 2009). Even motor preparation, a non-conscious process, reflects the brain’s readiness for action and is closely linked to our intentions and choices.
In summary, human decision-making is a complex interplay of conscious reasoning and powerful, often hidden, non-conscious and unconscious influences. Recognising this multi-layered architecture is crucial for comprehensively understanding human behaviour.
10. Conclusion: Towards an Integrated Understanding of the Mind
The human mind is a complex, multi-layered system, with conscious experience representing only a small part of its overall activity. Distinguishing between the unconscious, non-conscious and pre-conscious is not just a matter of semantics, but a vital step towards a more precise, empirically grounded understanding of mental processes. Although the term ‘subconscious’ is still used in everyday language, it has limited scientific value, and more specific terminology is favoured.
From the historical insights of Freudian psychoanalysis to the cutting-edge methodologies of modern neuroscience and cognitive psychology, our understanding of these hidden mental realms continues to evolve. Neuroimaging techniques such as EEG, ERP and fMRI, as well as sophisticated behavioural paradigms such as masked-prime tasks and reaction time experiments, provide invaluable tools for investigating processes that are inaccessible to direct introspection. Theoretical frameworks such as Global Workspace Theory and Predictive Coding offer compelling models of how conscious experience emerges from, and interacts with, a vast substrate of non-conscious processing.
Ultimately, recognising the profound and pervasive influence of these unseen mental forces on our perception, emotional regulation, decision-making and overall behaviour is fundamental to a holistic understanding of the human mind. Ongoing interdisciplinary research into the architecture of awareness promises to reveal more about the extraordinary complexity and adaptive capabilities of human cognition.
Mangold Observation Studio
The advanced software suite for sophisticated sensor data-driven observational studies with comprehensive data collection and analysis capabilities.

References
Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signalling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 410-422. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2648
Baars, B. J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge University Press.
Baars, B. J. (2002). The conscious access hypothesis: Origins and recent evidence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(1), 47-52. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01819-2
Baars, B. J. (2005). Global workspace theory of consciousness: toward a cognitive neuroscience of human experience. Progress in Brain Research, 150, 45-53. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(05)50004-9
Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (1999). The unbearable automaticity of being. American Psychologist, 54(7), 462-479. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.462
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181-204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477
Dehaene, S., & Changeux, J. P. (2011). Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron, 70(2), 200-227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2011.03.018
Dehaene, S., Naccache, L., Le Clec’H, G., Koechlin, E., Mueller, M., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., van de Moortele, P. F., & Le Bihan, D. (1998). Imaging unconscious semantic priming. Nature, 395(6702), 597-600. https://doi.org/10.1038/26967
Freud, S. (1915). The unconscious. In J. Strachey (Ed.), The Standard Edition of the Complete Psychological Works of Sigmund Freud, Volume XIV (1914-1916): On the History of the Psycho-Analytic Movement, Papers on Metapsychology, and Other Works (pp. 159-215). Hogarth Press.
Friston, K. (2005). A theory of cortical responses. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 360(1456), 815-836. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2005.1622
Harrison, N. A., Kreibig, S. D., & Critchley, H. D. (2013). A two-way road: Efferent and afferent pathways of autonomic activity in emotion. In J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Handbook of psychophysiology (4th ed., pp. 82-102). Cambridge University Press.
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Kouider, S., & Dehaene, S. (2007). Levels of processing during non-conscious perception: A critical review of visual masking. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1481), 857-870. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2007.2093
LeDoux, J. E. (1996). The emotional brain: The mysterious underpinnings of emotional life. Simon and Schuster.
Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential): The unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain, 106(3), 623-642. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623
Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1994). The myth of repressed memory: False memories and allegations of sexual abuse. St. Martin’s Press.
Luck, S. J. (2014). An introduction to the event-related potential technique (2nd ed.). MIT Press.
Reber, A. S. (1989). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118(3), 219-235. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.118.3.219
Schacter, D. L. (1987). Implicit memory: History and current status. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 13(3), 501-518. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.13.3.501
Vallar, G., & Perani, D. (1986). The anatomy of spatial neglect. In M. Jeannerod (Ed.), Neurophysiological and neuropsychological aspects of spatial neglect (pp. 205-221). North-Holland.
Weiskrantz, L. (1986). Blindsight: A case study and implications. Oxford University Press.
Whalen, P. J., Rauch, S. L., Etcoff, N. L., McInerney, S. C., Lee, M. B., & Jenike, M. A. (1998). Masked presentations of emotional facial expressions modulate amygdala activity without explicit knowledge. Journal of Neuroscience, 18(1), 411-418. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-01-00411.1998